Download Mundari Book

 

since it behave as a single unit. This compounding, incidentally, also has take morpho-phonemic change.
However, there may be a number of occasions where examples like/ nimin/have been referred to as ‘words’ or even the word like ‘suffixed’ has been used in place of ‘juxtaposed’ and so on. In such cases only the familiarity with the terms has been the main reason to have been employed than technicality. Therefore, reference to word would mean a root or a plus one or more affixes or even juxtaposed postpositions as well as a free like a particle.
2.1. Substantives: Such root words which take genetive suffixes and postpositions are substantives. Substantives are three-animate nouns, inanimate nouns and personal pronouns.
2.1.1. Animate nouns: In addition to the genetive affixes and postpositions, they also take suffixes for dual and plural, /kiN/ and /ko/ respectively, and are marked by them in the verbal phrase in case of third person subject or object.
2.1.2. Inanimate nouns: They differ from the animate in the way that the inanimates do not generally take suffixes for number and are marked in the verbal phrase. Exceptionally, when the duality or plurality is to be emphasized upon, e.g., /oa?/ ‘house’ /ora?-ki/ ‘two houses’ or when objects are enumerated, e.g., /buluko/, sunumko/, /cauliko/, i.e., salt, oil, rice etc., they may take such suffixes.
2.1.3. Personal pronouns: The personal pronouns differ from the other two by not occurring as a subject, apart from being marked in the verbal phrase. However, when pronominal subject i semphasized it can occur independently with free-form marker /a/ as prefix and emphasis marker /ge/ as suffix. Pronouns denoting theinanimates do not have dual and plural. The forms of personal pronouns are tabulated below:

Sg.
du.
pl.
1. ai
alaN (incl.)
abu (incl.)
ali (excl.)
ale (excle.)
2. am
aben
ape
3. ae
aki
ako

The formation of all the personal pronouns is made by prefixing a non-personal /a-/, which is a free-form marker. The rest part of each form is used as a subject or object as the case may be.
The other function of the contracted forms is to denote possession, e.g., setaiko ‘my dogs’. This type of construction without a genetive suffix is generally used for denoting kinship, otherwise: apuma? o·a? ‘your father’s house’, where a? is a genetive suffix.
2.1.4. Derivations: All the nouns can again be differetiated by being either action noun or non-action noun. Action noun are connected with an action and are adopted from the verb roots, e.g., hasu ‘pain or to pain’, rakab ‘ a climb or to climb’ , kuli ‘question or to put question’. The non-action nouns are imply the names of anything. All the action nouns are inanimate and do not take number suffixes. They are the substitutes for infinitives.
 There are three chief morphological processes for derivation of nouns: suffixation, infixation and
compounding -
(i) Suffixation -
(a) a? is suffixed to adjective roots to denote a thing with that quality, e.g., pun·i ‘white >pun·ia? ‘
a white thing’.
(b) ea? suffixed to a verb root for nominalization, e.g., her /to sow > hera? ‘that which is sown’; jom ‘to eat’ > jomea? ‘eatable’.
The formation of ea? is by suffixing genetive /a?/to/e/, possibly relative participal marker.
Simply a? can be added even to participal stems, as / olked/ ‘having written’, making it/ olkeda?/ ‘that, which some one has written’.
 

 

Previous

Next

top

 
Mundari Index Page
 
FeedBack | Contact Us | Home
ciil grammar footer