Download Mao Naga Book

 


 
35 c. ni idu-i takoo adi  cü vu-e
35d.  ni idu takoo adi vue-i
‘where4 [was] it that3 you [sg.]1 went3 yesterday2 ?’

Notice that the individuator -i can be attached not only to the verbal gerund, but also to the verb, the temporal sentential adverb idu ‘yesterday’ and the subject noun ni, irrespective of which of the four constituents it is suffixed to, -i covers all the five in its scope viz., ni, idu, takoo, adicü and vu.
-i, the individuator marks the fact that both the speaker and the hearer have seen and know about the referent of the noun it attaches to [see 3.3.4 for more details]. But idu-i ‘yesterday’ in the above example does not refer to wider in scope, refers rather to the fact that the speaker has seen the subject noun ni ‘you [sg.]’ do the action identified by the nonfinite verb takoo ‘going’ at a time signalled by idu ‘yesterday’. This is clearer in a sentence like
 
36 ni1 idu-i2 adi3 so-we4  
  what3 were4 you[sg.]1 doing4 yesterday2 ?

where, clearly -i in idu-i does not signal a yesterday which both of the interlocutors have seen and know about, but rather the fact that the speaker saw the referent of ni ‘you [sg.]’ do something yesterday and today wants to know what exactly it was that ni ‘you [sg.]’ was doing. In 37 and 37a respectively.
 
37 pfo-i1 cüdu-no2  ta-ie3
  when2 did3 he1 go away3 ?
       
37 a.  pfo1 cüdu-no2  ta-ie-i3


-i has the privilege of occurring either with the subject noun pfo ‘he’ or with the finite verb ta ‘go away’. The speaker of this sentence knows that the referent of the subject left or rather saw him leave in fact, but has forgotten the time/date of departure. On the other hand, the scope of gender affixes, derivational prefixes, for instance, does not range beyond morphology.


Gender suffix -krü.
 
38 a. tu-krü ‘cow’

b.

vo-krü ‘female pig’

c.

sohć-krü  ‘female buffalo’
 
d. si-krü ‘female dog’
e. ihĩ-krü ‘she-goat’

gender suffix -lo [from khelo]
 
39 a.  si-lo ‘male dog’

b.

 vo-lo ‘male pig’

derivational prefix

verb to adverb
40 a. zhü ‘be good’
40 b. ma-zhü ‘well’
41 a. shü ‘be bad’
41 b. ma-shü ‘badly’

noncausative verb to causative verb
 
42 a.  avi ‘to escape’
42 b.  ko-vi  ‘to cause to escape’
43 a. ta ‘to go’
43 b. ma-ta ‘to cause to go’
44 a. ba ‘to wear [as shawl]’
44 b. ma-ba ‘to cause to wear [as shawl]’
45 a. ‘to write’
45 b. mo-rü ‘to cause to write’

-we the habitual aspect marker is another example of this limited membership set
3.0.3.2.

 

The second type, the opposite of the first, is a morphosyntactic nonword and interphrasal and phonological word. This is statistically the largest type. All lexical units exemplify this type.
 
oni ‘festival’ odzü ‘water’
larübvü

 

‘book’

 

pfo

 

‘she; he’

 

 

Previous   

Next

Top

 
Mao Naga Index Page
 
FeedBack | Contact Us | Home
ciil grammar footer